大杜鹃雌鸟鸣声的日节律
作者:
作者单位:

1.生物多样性与生态工程教育部重点实验室,北京师范大学生命科学学院 北京 100875;2.深圳市龙华区同胜学校 深圳 518109

基金项目:

国家自然科学基金项目(No. 31601868)


Daily Vocal Pattern of Female Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus)
Author:
Affiliation:

1.Ministry of Education Key Laboratory for Biodiversity and Ecological Engineering,College of Life Sciences,Beijing Normal University;2.Shenzhen Tongsheng School

  • 摘要
  • | |
  • 访问统计
  • |
  • 参考文献 [1]
  • |
  • 相似文献
  • | | |
  • 文章评论
    摘要:

    本文描述了三个地区大杜鹃(Cuculus canorus)雌鸟鸣声的日节律。通过在北京野鸭湖湿地公园(2017年5月7日至7月8日)、辽宁辽河口自然保护区(2018年6月28日至7月29日)和吉林大岗子林场(2018年5月17日至7月10日)分别布设自动录音机10台、10台和8台,连续记录繁殖季大杜鹃的鸣声。共录到2 777次大杜鹃雌鸟的鸣叫。其中,1 431次来自北京野鸭湖湿地公园,1 222次来自辽宁辽河口自然保护区,124次来自吉林大岗子林场。野鸭湖湿地公园和辽河口自然保护区的大杜鹃雌鸟在日出时段出现鸣叫的高峰,而大岗子林场的大杜鹃在日出和日落时段各有一个鸣叫的高峰。通过比较大杜鹃雌鸟鸣声的日节律和大杜鹃产卵时间的日节律,本研究认为在上述地区大杜鹃雌鸟鸣叫的功能更可能是宣示领域和吸引异性,而非恐吓宿主。本研究有助于进一步了解大杜鹃雌鸟鸣声的功能。

    Abstract:

    In this study, the daily vocal pattern of female Common Cuckoos (Cuculus canorus) were described. Recordings were made using 28 passive acoustic recorders during breeding season in Wild Duck Lake Wetland Park, Beijing (10 recorders, from 7th May to 8th July in 2017), Liaohe Delta Nature Reserve, Liaoning (10 recorders, from 28th June to 29th July in 2018), and Dagangzi Forest Farm, Jilin (8 recorders, from 17th May to 10th July). A total of 2 777 female calls (Fig. 1) were detected, with 1 431 calls from Wild Duck Lake Wetland Park, 1 222 calls from Liaohe Delta Nature Reserve, and 124 calls from Dagangzi Forest Farm. The females in Wild Duck Lake Wetland Park and Liaohe Delta Nature Reserve showed a similar pattern of daily vocal activity, reaching a sharp peak in the dawn (Fig. 2a, b); while females in Dagangzi Forest Farm showed two peaks of daily vocal activity, with peak activity occurring around early morning and dusk (Fig. 2c). Through comparing these daily vocal patterns with daily egg-laying pattern (Fig. 2d), we suggest that the function of female call in the study populations is to advertise territory and attract mates, rather than misdirect host defenses. This study helps to understand the function of the female Common Cuckoo call.

    参考文献
    Davies N B. 2015. Cuckoo: Cheating by Nature. London: Bloomsbury. Davies N B, Brooke M D. 1988. Cuckoos versus reed warblers: adaptations and counteradaptations. Animal Behaviour, 36(1): 262–284. Deng Z Q, Lloyd H, Xia C W, et al. 2019. Components of variation in female common cuckoo calls. Behavioural Processes, 158: 106–112. Erritz?e J, Mann C F, Brammer F P, et al. 2012. Cuckoos of the World. London: Christopher Helm. Feeney W E, Welbergen J A, Langmore N E. 2012. The frontline of avian brood parasite-host coevolution. Animal Behaviour, 84(1): 3–12. Fugo T. 2003. Co-evolutionary dynamics of egg appearance in avian brood parasitism. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 5(3): 345–362. Helgesen I M, Hamblin S, Hurd P L. 2013. Does cheating pay? Re-examining the evolution of deception in a conventional signalling game. Animal Behaviour, 86(6): 1215–1224. Hutchinson J M C. 2002. Two explanations of the dawn chorus compared: how monotonically changing light levels favour a short break from singing. Animal Behaviour, 64(4): 527–539. Liang W. 2017. Crafty cuckoo calls. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 1(10): 1427–1428. Ma L K, Yang C C, Liang W. 2018. Hawk mimicry does not reduce attacks of cuckoos by highly aggressive hosts. Avian Research, 9(1): 35. Moksnes A, Foss?y F, R?skaft E, et al. 2013. Reviewing 30 years of studies on the Common Cuckoo accumulated knowledge and future perspectives. Chinese Birds, 4(1): 3–14. M?ller A P, Morelli F, Mousseau T A, et al. 2016. The number of syllables in Chernobyl cuckoo calls reliably indicate habitat, soil and radiation levels. Ecological Indicators, 66: 592–597. M?ller A P, Morelli F, Tryjanowski P. 2017. Cuckoo folklore and human well-being: cuckoo calls predict how long farmers live. Ecological Indicators, 72: 766–768. Moskát C, Hauber M E. 2019. Sex-specific responses to simulated territorial intrusions in the common cuckoo: a dual function of female acoustic signaling. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 73(5): 60. Payne R B. 2005. The Cuckoos. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Seel D C. 1973. Egg-laying in the cuckoo. British Birds, 66: 528–535. Soler M. 2014. Long-term coevolution between avian brood parasites and their hosts. Biological Reviews, 89(3): 688–704. Stoddard M C, Kilner R M. 2013. The past, present and future of 'cuckoos versus reed warblers'. Animal Behaviour, 85(4): 693–699. Szamado S. 2000. Cheating as a mixed strategy in a simple model of aggressive communication. Animal Behaviour, 59(1): 221–230. Thomas R J, Szekely T, Cuthill I C, et al. 2002. Eye size in birds and the timing of song at dawn. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 269(1493): 831–837. Xia C W, Deng Z Q, Lloyd H, et al. 2019. The function of three main call types in common cuckoo. Ethology, 125(9): 652–659. Yoo S, Kim H N, Lee J W, et al. 2020. Seasonal and diurnal patterns of population vocal activity in avian brood parasites. Ibis, doi.org/10.1111/ibi.12741. York J E, Davies N B. 2017. Female cuckoo calls misdirect host defences towards the wrong enemy. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 1(10): 1520–1525. Yu J P, Xing X Y, Jiang Y L, et al. 2017. Alarm call-based discrimination between common cuckoo and Eurasian sparrowhawk in a Chinese population of great tits. Ethology, 123(8): 542–550. 黄婉萍, 徐姝婷, 梁伟, 等. 2017. 大鹰鹃鸣声的日节律. 动物学杂志, 52(6): 945–953.
    相似文献
    引证文献
引用本文

龚玉杰,王萌萌,周冰,邓竹青,何衍,夏灿玮.2020.大杜鹃雌鸟鸣声的日节律.动物学杂志,55(5):560-565.

复制
文章指标
  • 点击次数:
  • 下载次数:
  • HTML阅读次数:
  • 引用次数:
历史
  • 收稿日期:2020-04-29
  • 最后修改日期:2020-08-23
  • 录用日期:2020-08-20
  • 在线发布日期: 2020-10-13